Skip to main content

Decision-making at the limit of viability: the Austrian neonatal choice context

Abstract

Background

We aimed to explore the shared decision-making context at the limit of viability (weeks 22–25 of gestation) through analyzing neonatologist’s communication strategies with parents and their possible impact on survival and neurodevelopmental impairment (NDI) outcomes.

Methods

A mixed methods approach was applied where a systematic literature search and in-depth semi-structured interviews with five heads of neonatology departments and one clinical ethicist from the Austrian context were integrated into a literature review. The aim was to identify decision practice models and the choice context specific to Austria.

Results

Professional biases, parental understanding, and the process of information giving were identified as aspects possibly influencing survival and NDI outcomes. Institutions create self-fulfilling prophecies by recommending intensive/palliative care based upon their institutional statistics, yet those vary considerably among high-income countries. Labelling an extremely preterm (EP) infant by the gestational week was shown to skew the estimates for survival while the process of information giving was shown to be subject to framing effect and other cognitive biases.

Conclusion

Communication strategies of choice options to parents may have an impact on the way parents decide and hence also on the outcomes of EP infants.

Peer Review reports

Background

Globally, less than 1 % of all pregnant women give birth extremely preterm (EP), before the completion of 28 weeks of pregnancy [1]. In Austria, 350 infants were born extremely preterm in 2016, accounting for 0.4% of all births [2]. Despite these relatively small numbers, extreme prematurity is the leading cause of infant death [3]. It is also potentially related to short and long-term morbidity accounting for almost 45% of children with cerebral palsy, 35% with visual impairment, and 25% with cognitive or hearing impairment in the US [4].

The ways in which EP births are currently managed include prevention, preparation for the delivery, as well as intensive and palliative (comfort) care treatment options post-delivery. It is particularly between weeks 22 to 25 of gestation - the limit of viability, when shared decision-making with parents concerning intensive and comfort care options is at stake. The intensive care options include the application of surfactant therapy, intubation, and supportive ventilation, while comfort care options aim at improving an infant’s quality of life (QoL), treating symptoms, and minimizing pain and suffering [5].

Limit of viability is the point in foetal development at which the EP infant has a chance for extra-uterine survival [6]. This definition of the limit of viability is changing over time due to improvements in treatment and care. These improvements lead also to better survival and neurodevelopmental impairment (NDI) outcomes that differ between institutions and countries [7]. However, there is a considerable consensus among high income countries that with intensive care, most infants born after 25 weeks and 0 days (25 + 0) of gestational age (GA) will survive, while there is a little chance for survival and survival without severe impairment in infants born below 22 + 0 weeks of GA [6]. The probability of survival and survival without impairment increases significantly over these few weeks (22 + 0 to 25 + 6) [8]. In the context of intensive and comfort care, determining this point with as much precision as possible is important in order to prevent inflicting unnecessary burden on the infant and the family on the one hand, yet to give sufficient chances for survival to the infant on the other hand.

Finding the agreement during shared decision-making with parents of EP infants before and after the delivery is one of the key tasks of the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (NICU) team. This decision-making is occurring at the backdrop of a specific cultural, socio-economic, and religious context and because the content that needs to be communicated is highly sensitive, it places even more emphasis on the form of communication. However, neonatologist’s communication strategies with parents in preparation for delivery as well as after delivery are not addressed in current guidelines (GLs). The fact that the form of communication has a significant impact on the person’s decision-making is well supported by research from behavioural sciences [9] and hence it is assumed that the same applies to the NICU context of choice. The design in which the options of choices are communicated is assumed to have an impact on the way parents decide and hence on the survival and NDI outcomes of EP infants.

In this review, we aimed to explore the shared decision-making context through reviewing the general literature and comparing it to the specific Austrian context in which we conducted qualitative interviews with the heads of NICUs and a clinical ethicist. This paper is a shortened version of one part of a larger health technology assessment (HTA), which particularly focuses on the analysis of the neonatologist’s communication strategies with parents and their possible impact on survival and NDI outcomes of EP infants. The HTA was conducted to provide decision support for resource planning of NICUs in Austria and additionally included the assessment of outcomes, resources, and ethical challenges [10, 11].

Methods

A mixed methods approach was applied. In the first step, a systematic literature search was conducted to identify the most relevant sources applying the MIP (Methodology, Issue, Participants) question and inclusion criteria as listed in the Table 1. Secondly, interviews with the heads of departments of neonatology of five perinatal care centres and a clinical ethicist working at a NICU were conducted to gather data specific to the Austrian neonatal context. Data from the systematic search as well as the interviews were integrated into a literature review. Each part of the text thus includes data from the literature review that reflects the general state of the neonatal literature and supports it by the context specific data from interviews (presented in the running text or standing alone as quotes).

Table 1 Methodology, Issue, Participants (MIP), and inclusion criteria for systematic search

Systematic literature search

The systematic literature search was conducted in the period between 21.06.2017 and 23.06.2017 in the following databases:

  • Medline via Ovid

  • Embase

  • The Cochrane Library

  • CRD (DARE, NHS-EED, HTA)

  • PsychInfo

  • CINAHL

The systematic search was not limited to a specific study design, but it was limited to specific languages (German and English) and the publication period 1990–2017. The reason for limiting the year of publication to 1990 was to limit the volume of hits and to focus on the most up-to-date decision models in neonatal care. After deduplication, overall 385 citations were included. The specific search strategy employed can be found in the availability of data and materials section. In addition, a hand search of literature (web-search) was performed and yielded additional 43 sources, resulting in overall 428 hits. 80 publications were finally selected for the analysis (see Fig. 1: PRISMA Flow Diagram).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow chart of study selection (PRISMA Flow Diagram)

The data retrieved from the 80 publications selected for the analysis were not extracted into extraction tables, but were analysed according to content analysis where all the topics relevant to the theme of biases in neonatal decision-making were included. In terms of literature selection, one author (MS), reviewed the abstracts and included/excluded them according to the MIP question. The second author (KH) reviewed the included abstracts. Any disagreements were resolved through discussion.

Interviews

Six semi-structured interviews with five heads of neonatal departments (out of the total of seven departments in Austria) and one clinical ethicist working in a NICU were conducted with the aim of exploring the choice context specific to Austria. Ethics approval was not requested for interviewing stakeholders based on section 15b, subsection 3a, of the Viennese Law on health institutions.

Interviews were conducted in person or via telephone. All interviews were audio-recorded and afterwards transcribed verbatim. Verbal consent was given by all interview participants prior to recording, audio proof of verbal consent has been collected. An example of the verbatim transcript can be found in the code tree in Table 2.

Table 2 Code tree

The interview duration ranged from 30 min to 60 min, one single interview lasted 1 h and 40 min. Two researchers conducted and coded the interviews. Interviews were held in English and in some cases, clarifications were phrased in German.

Prior to the data analysis, written transcripts and summaries were sent to the interview participants to confirm the exactness of their quotes. At the time of the HTA’s external review, near to final versions were sent again for final confirmation. If necessary, changes were made in the transcripts and summaries.

To analyse the transcripts, a combination of open coding and structured thematic analysis was applied [12]. This analysis was performed beginning with fragmentation and open-coding of each transcript. Thereby, every fragment received a code such as a word or a short sentence to identify themes.

The main codes and themes were organised in a code-tree. In addition, the themes from the interview topic list served as a structural GL to analyse the interviews. Subsequently, the results of all interviews were edited and common themes and codes integrated. Data analysis was performed using the coding software Atlas.ti (Version 8).

Results: communication with parents - biases influencing outcomes

The following section is structured in categories of professional biases, parental understanding, and information-giving and choice.

Professional biases

Communication with parents is shaped by the perceptions and biases of health care professionals in NICU teams. This can, for instance, affect the presentation of treatment options. Parents have to decide based on the content – information provided by the NICU professionals, and the form – the way in which NICU professionals communicate to them. These parental decisions then affect institutional statistics, which in return influence the information provided in the future. Providers need to acknowledge their professional biases, in particular: institutional, personal, and informational bias [13].

Institutional bias

There are differences in results of neonates between countries and more importantly between institutions (see Table 3) [14]. One of the explanations comes down to the role of social norms and institutional biases. As suggested by Lantos 2009, “the policy that limits treatment for infants born at 24 weeks of gestation will lead to low survival rates for those infants. The low survival rates will seem to justify and validate the policy, even if the true causal relationship runs in the other direction” [18]. Such a path creates self-fulfilling prophecies because such defaults both reflect on the social norms as well as create them [19].

Table 3 Recommendations According to Week of Gestation as of 2015 (German speaking countries update) [14]

With respect to the institutional differences as well as the differences in social norms, Interviewee 5 describes a situation of a Swiss couple delivering an EP infant in Austria. While there is a consensus among all NICUs in Austria that intensive care starts by default in 24th week of gestation and if parents decide so, intensive care is also routinely delivered in 23rd week [15], Interviewee 5 states that in Switzerland and the Netherlands, by default, comfort care is delivered in 23rd week of gestation, because.

“they are really more conservative… I remember we had a Swiss couple travelling through who did not want to deliver their baby here, but then she had a premature rapture of membranes at 23 weeks plus something…and for her, it was completely normal not to go for this baby. Then we talked a lot with this family and at the end, we convinced them to actively go for this baby and they had a wonderful outcome.”

The presence of such default bias is also supported by an RCT with adult volunteers that studied the impact of defaults in the NICU context. Participants were randomised to receive either resuscitation or comfort care as the delivery room management default option for a hypothetical delivery of an infant at 23 weeks of GA. Those participants that were told that the default option was resuscitation were more likely to opt for resuscitation and the effect persisted on multivariate regression analysis [20]. The default option created a norm that the participants had the tendency to follow.

Personal bias

Not only institutional, but also personal biases may have an impact on the assessment of viability by the NICU professionals and subsequently, on the survival and NDI outcomes. This can be observed on surveys and assessments of hypothetical scenarios, where the variety of conclusions points to the presence of personal attitudes and biases. While NICU professionals in one Australian narrative review underestimated survival and positive outcomes of infants between weeks 22 and 26 of GA [21], in other Australian and US surveys, they also overestimated major neurosensory disability at both week 24 and 28 of GA [22] and long term disability [23]. To the contrary, however, UK NICU professionals in a questionnaire survey overestimated infant survival and underestimated intact infant survival rate [24]. Also, a Finish survey found that NICU professionals with the longest years’ working experience were reluctant to administer steroids to mothers at the lowest weeks of GA to speed up the process of development of the infant [25]. Furthermore, a US study equally revealed the personal biases of the health care professionals by pointing to the correlation between the obstetricians’ willingness to intervene and the periviable outcomes [26]. In this way, personal biases influence the chances for survival of extremely premature infants.

Informational bias

NICU professionals make their decisions also based upon their own informational biases. Firstly, reliance on how the baby looks right after the delivery is one of the strategies of neonatologists for predicting survival estimates. Same was suggested in the interviews that,

“if the 23 weeker doesn’t have any vital signs (and the parents don’t want us to do, really everything), comfort care comes in ... the baby shows what to do.” (Interviewee 2)

However, a possible issue with overreliance on early clinical signs was shown in an Australian study where the neonatologists’ ability to predict survival based on appearance and early response was poor. Videos on ten EP infants were shown to 17 neonatal fellows at 20 s, 2 min, and 5 min after birth. Predictive ability of the neonatal fellows was inaccurate and the level of experience did not affect accuracy of the prediction of survival [27].

Secondly, labelling a periviable infant by the gestational week was shown to skew the estimates for survival and uncover the informational bias of the NICU teams. In a Canadian survey, relying on GA alone led to incorrect assessment of outcomes compared to when the preterm infant was described by its prognosis [28]. Furthermore, in a US survey among obstetrician-gynaecologists, GA was weighted more heavily than parental resuscitation preferences [26], even though the ultrasound evidence of GA may vary by 15% and the gestational weight estimate by 2 weeks [29].

Reliance of on very early ultrasound evidence for GA measurement was also reported in the interviews. Interviewee 5 states that they have confidence in the GA measurement if it comes from the obstetricians within the hospital, because.

“…with our obstetricians…normally, we know very well the exact date of birth because most of our NICU patients have a very early ultrasound…So, normally, we have very good data there.”

Furthermore, educational interventions show that personal and informational biases of NICU professionals can improve. A survey examining the relationship between knowledge of participants and their attitude towards resuscitation showed knowledge gaps. After the educational presentation, NICU professionals changed their attitudes and were more prone to resuscitate at all GAs regardless of parental wishes than before [30]. Another survey with hypothetical case scenarios showed that after the educational intervention, respondents improved significantly in the accuracy of their survival and disability estimates [23]. The presence of professional biases – institutional, personal, or informational – thus needs to be acknowledged as it unavoidably influences the survival and NDI outcomes of EP infants in respective institutions.

Parental understanding

Because preterm infants cannot communicate their preferences autonomously, decisions must be made by proxy [17]. If needed, this surrogate role can be played also by the NICU team or by a societal body such as an ethics committee, or a court of law [31]. In the Austrian NICU decision-making context, ethics committees are in place in all the centres included in the analysis and while some are organized ad hoc by the head of the respective NICU in challenging cases (as stated by Interviewee 5), others have established standard operating procedures that are being followed (as stated by Interviewees 4 and 6). Especially in the grey zone, however, it is the legal guardians that, ideally, give consent with the help of NICU professionals in a shared decision-making procedure. Parental decision-making is, however challenging, as Interviewee 4 puts it,

“I think it’s a real big problem because in this week, the parents must say ‘yes’ or ‘no’ and they must live with this decision.”

At the same time, the NICU team needs to work in accordance with the parents to the extent possible, as Interviewee 1 suggests,

“I’m very strongly emphasising this for all our working groups in the NICU that we always have to be in accordance with the parents. If we lose the parents, we lose the infant somehow as well.”

When communicating with parents, however, one size does not fit all as different parents have different information needs. A recurring theme in the literature calls for parents to be provided with the most accurate prognosis and care options possible in order to make a competent decision [13, 32]. Parents, however, seem to have needs that are so heterogeneous that acting by the principle is not sufficient. As stated by the Interviewee 5 and suggested in the literature, some parents require all the detail possible [13], while others would not be influenced at all by the information provided because of their own value frameworks in place [33].

When making a decision, parents are put under extreme stress [34] and some prefer that the competent NICU professional decides on their behalf [9, 21]. Also, due to the recent dramatic demographic change and the rise in the migrant population, the Austrian NICU professionals face families who have never encountered the idea of shared decision-making and so as Interviewee 5 puts it,

“…we sometimes really have to fall back and make a paternalistic decision.”

Because parents differ in their capacity and need to understand, it is important that the NICU professionals try to capture the level of understanding of parents and identify their main concerns.

“We need to develop a sense of who these parents are in order to ensure effective communication for both sides.” (Interviewee 5)

Interviewee 2 further states that if the communication from the side of NICU professionals is personal and empathetic, it is nearly always possible to work with the parents.

“If you talk to them, if you have enough time for them, if you try to understand them, I think you won’t have a problem...We answer their questions, we talk about outcome, about survival, about major handicaps, we also talk about what will happen if the baby will come during the next days. If possible, we show them the neonatal intensive care unit...we describe what will happen, that the baby will need respiratory support, tube feeding, central venous line, and so on, and so on…”

As supported by both the literature and the interviews, the data communicated to the parents need to be personalized because parents have different information needs to be begin with.

Real life data and psychological support

There is, however, also a discrepancy between the information that parents can be provided in the NICU and the information that parents would actually need to make a better informed decision. Parents typically receive information about outcomes, prognosis, and care options, however, to make an informed decision, parents would need to know the translation of the numbers they are given into their real life. They would like to find out how the prognosis would influence their family situation, what QoL their child would have, or whether their child would be happy [13].

In case of EP infants, the term QoL is particularly debatable. While there are generic QoL measures (such as 36-Item Short Form (SF-36) or EuroQol five dimension scale (EQ-5D) [35]) and health-related quality of life (HRQoL) measures developed for adults, and older children, there are no established measures for EP infants [36]. There are even family QoL measures in place such as the Family Quality of Life Scale (FQOL) [37]. For EP infants, Boss, Kinsman, and Donohue 2012 suggest that HRQoL metrics on emotional and social functioning for adults and paediatric patients “could be adapted to focus on the role of a neonate in the context of a family unit, as with caregiver ratings of their ability to bond with infant” [36]. Furthermore, they suggest that physical domains such as “pain, energy/neurobehavior, sleep and physical symptoms could be adapted from existing NICU metrics” for instance, the Neonatal Infant Pain Scale, Neonatal Care Unit Network Neurobehavioral Scale, Polysomnography, or Nursing-Child Assessment Feeding Scale [36]. Standardizing the EP infant’s QoL or HRQoL measures could prove beneficial in preventing the neonatal QoL discussion to be narrowed down to analysing only the infant’s physical and cognitive impairments.

Janvier, Barrington, and Farlow 2014 further suggest that parents should be given reassurance about coping strategies, for instance that after experiencing a severe complication, patients tend to return to their baseline QoL after 24 months [13]. Furthermore, they should receive information on the risk of developing psychological problems because there is a substantial increase in depression, anxiety, and financial stress after the birth of a preterm infant, which, however, generally decreases over time [13]. They should also be given information that there is an increase in family cohesion, less conflict than in typical families, and no increase in divorce in families with preterm infants [13].

The content of psychological support was not discussed in the interviews, but with respect to psychological support for families with EP infants, the Interviewees reported a homogenous use across all five perinatal care centres. All centres reported that there was a psychological support for parents. In the centre of Interviewee 5, one psychologist supported the family through the entire hospital stay, whereas in the centre of Interviewee 1, the family was transferred from the obstetrics psychologist to the NICU psychologist. Furthermore, Interviewee 3 suggested that psychological support ideally comes in before birth, when both, a neonatologist as well as a psychologist talk to the parents about the possible courses of action. However, Interviewee 5 reminded that counselling also comes from nurses, who spend a lot of time with parents at the bedside.

Because parents come from different backgrounds and have different information needs, the communication from the side of NICU professionals needs to adjust. Moreover, apart from medical data about outcomes, psychological support and real life psychological data about the impact of having an EP infant need to be communicated to the parents [13].

Information-giving and choice biases

When communicating with parents, no neutral or uniformed tools can solve the problem of the biases of NICU professionals and the individuality of parents. For that reason, doctors, counsellors, as well as nurses need to be aware of their own biases and they need to use their best judgment to provide balanced information to parents that is also personalized. As the Interviewee 5 puts it that is their NICU,

“parents get an idea of the medical data as well as an idea of what the NICU team thinks is worth-while doing (not with regards to resources and money, but in the interest of the EP infant and the family).”

In the process of passing the information to parents, however, different cognitive biases are at play (see Table 4), in particular, a framing effect.

Table 4 Cognitive biases and their influence on decision-making [13]

Framing effect

Framing effect is one of many cognitive biases that reveals that people react to a particular choice in different ways depending on the way it is presented. Sometimes, there seems to be a discrepancy between what NICU professionals think that they communicate to parents and what parents actually perceive. Structured interviews with mothers and counsellors revealed that mothers perceived the counselling about resuscitation of extremely premature infants directive, even though the majority of counsellors believed that mothers were given a choice of treatment options [38].

When communicating with parents, framing effect of how the data is presented is inevitable. Communicating proportional outcomes constitutes the majority of information that is being given to parents, however, many individuals do not understand percentages [13]. Patients tend to choose a procedure where the risk of death is described as 24 out of 100, but they tend not to choose the one where the risk is described as 120 out of 1000. Even though the risk is smaller in the latter, patients presumably tend not to choose that procedure because 120 is a larger number than 24 [13].

The question of framing effect did not come up in any of the interviews, but for the sake of better communication with parents, an instant translating system called Videodolmotsch that interacts with a real interpreter via a screen was used in the centre of Interviewees 5 and 6.

“,,,we always discuss with parents, of course, which is difficult if you are confronted with families who do not have the cultural, religious, or intellectual basis to decide. We still discuss it with them. We have a Videodolmetsch system where we at least try to show those parents, or pregnant women, what the odds of complications and NDIs are. But normally, it is like “Please do everything for our baby”.” (Interviewee 5)

The attitude of parents to save the EP infant at all costs is, as shown above, context specific as it is subject to the respective societal norms. The impact of framing seems to persist nonetheless as a randomized survey found a trend towards a framing effect on the treatment preference in NICU decision-making. Participants for whom the prognosis was framed as survival and non-disability rates were more likely to choose resuscitation than participants for whom prognosis was framed as mortality and disability rates [39]. Framing effect can also be seen when presenting a list of complications of an intervention, although there is also evidence against it [40]. When the list starts with the rarest and ends with them most common complication, patients tend to choose against the intervention. When presented in the opposite order, patients tend to choose for it [13]. Other cognitive biases that have an impact on the decision-making at the limit of viability are described in Table 4.

Discussion

This literature review outlined the current evidence on decision-making practices, especially on neonatologist’s communication strategies at the limit of viability. Data from the systematic literature search were supported by specific data from the interviews with Austrian heads of NICUs and a clinical ethicist. The overlap between the literature reviewed and the content of the semi-structured interviews is the main advance that this paper adds on the existing literature. The categories of biases found in the literature were also found to be present in the Austrian neonatal context, namely professional biases, parental understanding, and the process of information giving were identified as aspects possibly influencing survival and NDI outcomes.

Outcomes and biases

Especially within the grey zone, the difference in practices may lead to a difference in outcomes. The Swiss policy of routinely providing comfort care at 23 weeks of GA leads to low survival rates for those infants [17]. These outcomes then become part of institutional statistics that further influence counselling and so the outcomes based on historical data influence the present decision-making of parents and thus the future outcomes. Institutional biases such as this and other biases that are at play during shared decision-making processes (see Table 4) are understood to be in part responsible for the variation in outcomes between hospitals.

As behavioural sciences argue, the real decision-making involves the use of mental shortcuts (biases) and one ought not to perceive them as undesirable interferences with the rational decision-making process [41]. It is necessary to recognize the impact of these biases on the decision-making in NICUs as they are an inevitable part of the process that needs to be taken into account when developing GLs for shared decision-making procedures [13]. Good practice decision models need to include not only psychological support for the parents and ethics support for the NICU professionals that are already to some extent acknowledged, but also support for neonatologists in terms of communication strategies with parents as those may influence outcomes as well [42]. It is argued here that there is a need for trainings of NICU professionals aiming first at recognition of the biases and second, at the reduction of influence of their personal and informational biases on the parents.

Limitations

In terms of limitations to this literature review, we acknowledge, that firstly, we limited the search language to German and English, which presumably led to leaving out of some literature. Nonetheless, we consider the literature that we used to be robust enough to provide a good overview of the decision-making models in place.

Secondly, even though this literature review was based on a systematic literature search complemented by a thorough hand search, the way of reporting remained non-systematic because we could not find a common denominator that would allow for comparison between the variety of studies. Hence, also no quality assessment or risk of bias tools were used.

Conclusion

Communication strategies of choice options to parents may have an impact on the way parents decide and hence also on the survival and NDI outcomes of EP infants. Professional biases, parental understanding, and the process of information giving were identified as aspects possibly influencing outcomes. For these reasons, it is important to address the impact of communication in the management GLs of EP infants as well as in in-house trainings for NICU professionals.

Availability of data and materials

Search strategies as well as all data generated or analyzed during this study are included in this published report:

Stanak M, Hawlik K. Perinatal Care at the threshold of viability: Decision-making at the threshold of viability and ethical challenges at Neonatal Intensive Care Units (NICUs). LBI-HTA Project No.: 97b; 2017. Wien: Ludwig Boltzmann Institute for Health Technology Assessment.

Abbreviations

CRD:

Centre for Reviews and Dissemination

EP:

Extremely preterm

EQ-5D:

EuroQol five dimension scale

GA:

Gestational age

GL:

Guideline

HRQoL:

Health-related quality of life

HTA:

Health technology assessment

MIP:

Methodology, issue, participants

NDI:

Neurodevelopmental impairment

NICU:

Neonatal intensive care unit

QoL:

Quality of life

SF-36:

Medical outcomes study 36-item short form

References

  1. Glass HC, Costarino AT, Stayer SA, Brett CM, Cladis F, Davis PJ. Outcomes for Extremely Premature Infants. Anesth Analg. 2015;120(6):1337–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Statistik der natürlichen Bevölkerungsbewegung [database on the]. 2016. Available from: http://www.statistik.at/web_de/statistiken/menschen_und_gesellschaft/bevoelkerung/geborene/medizinische_und_sozialmedizinische_merkmale/index.html. [cited 25/10/2017]

  3. Patel RM. Short- and long-term outcomes for extremely preterm infants. Am J Perinatol. 2016;33(3):318–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Jarjour IT. Neurodevelopmental outcome after extreme prematurity: a review of the literature. Pediatr Neurol. 2015;52(2):143–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Catlin A, et al. Palliative and end-of-life Care for Newborns and Infants. Adv Neonatal Care. 2015;15(4):239–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Mercurio MR. Periviable birth (limit of viability). UpToDate Inc; 2017; Available from: https://www.uptodate.com/contents/periviable-birth-limit-of-viability. [cited 09/11/2017]

  7. Zeitlin J, Szamotulska K, Drewniak N, Mohangoo A, Chalmers J, Sakkeus L, et al. Preterm birth time trends in Europe: a study of 19 countries. BJOG Int J Obstet Gynaecol. 2013;120(11):1356–65.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Myrhaug HTBK, Hov L, Håvelsrud K, Reinar LM. Prognose for og oppfølging av ekstremt premature barn: En systematisk oversikt. Oslo: Norwegian Institute for Public Health; 2017.

  9. Sunstein CR. Nuding Health: Health Law and Behavioural economics,. Cohen G, Fernandez. Lynch H, Robertson CT, editors. Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press; 2016.

  10. Stanak M. Professional ethics: the case of neonatology. Med Health Care Philos.2019;22(2):231–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Stanak M, Hawlik K. Perinatal care at the threshold of viability part II: decision-making at the threshold of viability and ethical challenges at neonatal intensive care units (NICUs). Vienna: Ludwig Boltzmann Institute for Health Technology Assessment; 2017. Available from: http://eprints.hta.lbg.ac.at/1148/1/HTA-Projektbericht_Nr.97b.pdf. [cited 2017 12.01.2017]

    Google Scholar 

  12. CT DMA. Principles of social research. London: Open University Press; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Janvier A, Barrington K, Farlow B. Communication with parents concerning withholding or withdrawing of life-sustaining interventions in neonatology. Semin Perinatol. 2014;38(1):38–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Guillén Ú, et al. Guidelines for the Management of Extremely Premature Deliveries: a systematic review. Pediatrics. 2015;136(2):343–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Berger A, et al. Erstversorgung von Frühgeborenen an der Grenze der Lebensfähigkeit. Monatsschr Kinderheilkd. 2017;165:139–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Deutschen Gesellschaft für Gynäkologie und Geburtshilfe, Deutschen Gesellschaft für Kinder- und Jugendmedizin, Deutschen Gesellschaft für Perinatale Medizin, Akademie für Ethik in der Medizin, Gesellschaft für Neonatologie und Pädiatrische Intensivmedizin Frühgeborene an der Grenze der Lebensfähigkeit 2014.

  17. Berger TM, et al. Perinatal care at the limit of viability between 22 and 26 completed weeks of gestation in Switzerland. Swiss Med Wkly. 2011;141:w13280.

  18. Lantos J, Meadow W. Variation in treatment of infants born at the borderline of viability. Pediatrics. 2009;123:1588–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Rysavy MALL, Bell EF, Das A, Hintz SR, Stoll BJ, et al. Between-hospital variation in treatment and outcomes in extremely preterm infants. N Engl J Med. 2015;372(19):1801–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Haward MF, Murphy RO, Lorenz JM. Default options and neonatal resuscitation decisions. J Med Ethics. 2012;38(12):713–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Ireland S, Ray R, Larkins S, Woodward L. Factors influencing the care provided for periviable babies in Australia: a narrative review. Reprod Health. 2015;12:108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Boland RA, Davis PG, Dawson JA, Doyle LW. What are we telling the parents of extremely preterm babies? Aust NZ J Obstet Gynaecol. 2016;56:274–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Blanco F, Suresh G, Howard D, Soll RF. Ensuring accurate knowledge of prematurity outcomes for prenatal counseling. Pediatrics. 2005;115(4):e478–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Chan KL, Kean LJ, Marlow N. Staff views on the management of the extremely pretern infant. Eur J Obstet Gynecol Reprod Biol. 2006;128:142–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Taittonen L, Korhonen PH, Palomaki O, Luukkaala T, Tammela O. Opinions of the counselling, care and outcome of extremely premature birth among healthcare professionals in Finland. Acta Paediatr. 2013;103:262–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Tucker Edmonds B, McKenzie F, Farrow V, Raglan G, Schulkin J. A national survey of obstetricians’ attitudes toward and practice of periviable intervention. J Perinatol. 2015;35(5):338–43.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Brett JM, et al. Clinical assessment of extremely premature infants in the delivery room is a poor predictor of survival. Pediatrics. 2010;125(3):e5559–e564.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Dempsey EM, Barrington KJ. Diagnostic criteria and therapeutic interventions for the hypotensive very low birth weight infant. J Perinatol. 2006;26(11):677–81.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Kett JC. Prenatal consultation for extremely preterm neonates: ethical pitfalls and proposed solutions. J Clin Ethics. 2015;26(3):241–9.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Doucette S, Lemyre B, Daboval T, Dunn S, Akiki S, Barrowman N, et al. Effect of an educational presentation about extremely preterm infants on knowledge and attitudes of Health care providers. Am J Perinatol. 2017;34(10):982–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Wallner J. Careful planning of the decision-making process in neonatology: ethical orientation. Z Geburtsh Neonatol. 2009;213:147–54.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Batton DG. Committee on F, newborn. Clinical report--antenatal counseling regarding resuscitation at an extremely low gestational age. Pediatrics. 2009;124(1):422–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Boss RD, Hutton N, Sulpar LJ, West AM, Donohue PK. Values parents apply to decision-making regarding delivery room resuscitation for high-risk newborns. Pediatrics. 2008;122(3):583–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Gallagher K, Martin J, Keller M, Marlow N. European variation in decision-making and parental involvement during preterm birth. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2014;99(3):F245–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Coons SJ, Rao S, Keininger DL, Hays RD. A comparative review of generic quality-of-life instruments. PharmacoEconomics. 2000;17(1):13–35.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Boss RD, Kinsman HI, Donohue PK. Health-related quality of life for infants in the neonatal intensive care unit. J Perinatol. 2012;32:901.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Hoffman L, Marquis J, Poston D, Summers JA, Turnbull A. Assessing family outcomes: psychometric evaluation of the beach center family quality of life scale. J Marriage Fam. 2006;68(4):1069–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Keenan HT, Doron MW, Seyda BA. Comparison of mothers’ and counselors’ perceptions of predelivery counseling for extremely premature infants. Pediatrics. 2005;116(1):104–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Haward MF, Murphy RO, Lorenz JM. Message framing and perinatal decisions. Pediatrics. 2008;122(1):109–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Haward MF, John LK, Lorenz JM, Fischoff B. Effects of description of options on perantal perinatal decision-making. Pediatrics. 2012;129(5):891–901.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Halpern D. Inside the nudge unit. Lonson: Penguin; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Berger TM. Guidelines for the management of extremely preterm deliveries in the grey zone of viability between 23 and 24 weeks’ gestation vary widely in developed countries. Evid Based Med. 2015;20(6):227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are most thankful to Tarquin Mittermayr for his excellent help with the literature search.

Funding

This research was funded by the Ludwig Boltzmann Institute for Health Technology Assessment, Vienna, Austria. Both authors were employees of the institute at the time of writing of the study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MS drafted the outline of the study, initiated the literature search, and made the first draft of the manuscript. KH revised the outline of the study, reviewed the included literature, and contributed to every revision of the manuscript. Both authors approve of the submitted manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michal Stanak.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethics approval was not requested for interviewing stakeholders based on section 15b, subsection 3a, of the Viennese Law on health institutions. Verbal consent was given by all interview participants prior to recording, audio proof of verbal consent has been collected.

Consent for publication

No consent for publication was needed as all interview participants we anonymized.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Stanak, M., Hawlik, K. Decision-making at the limit of viability: the Austrian neonatal choice context. BMC Pediatr 19, 204 (2019). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12887-019-1569-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12887-019-1569-5

Keywords